Definition
of reading: Kenneth
Goodman (1967, 1988).
"Reading is a receptive language process. It
is a psycholinguistic guessing game. There is an essential interaction
between language and thought in reading. The writer
encodes thought
as language and the reader decodes language as thought.
"Proficient readers are both Efficient and Effective.
They are Effective in constructing meaning throughout
the reading process, and this meaning bears some level of agreement with
the original meaning of the author.
They are Efficient in using the least amount
of effort to achieve effectiveness. To accomplish this they maintain constant
focus on constructing the meaning throughout the process:
Competence Vs. Performance:
Competence = what readers are capable of doing. It results in the reader's
control of and flexibility in using the reading process
Performance = what we observe them to do. It is the observable result
of the competence.
Researchers would be committing a serious error if they equated what readers do with what they are capable of doing.
"The ability to read the written language at a reasonable rate with good comprehension has long been recognized to be as important as oral skills, if not more important." (Eskey 1970) (p. 1)
Reading research is just a little more than a hundred years old. Serious attempts at building explicit models of the reading process have a history of a little more than forty years. (Samuels & Kamil, p. 22)
That reading is not a passive, but rather an active, and in fact an
interactive, process has been recognized for some time in native language
reading but it is only recently that second/foreign language reading has
been viewed as an active rather than a passive process.
Early workin second language reading assumed a rather passive, bottom-up,
view of second language reading. It was viewed primarily as a decoding
process of reconstructing the author's intended meaning via recognizing
the printed letters and words, and building up a meaning for a text from
the smallest textual units at the bottom (letters and words) to larger
units at the top (phrases, clauses, links). Problems of SL reading and
reading comprehension were viewed as being essentially decoding problems,
deriving meaning from print.
In the early seventies, Goodman's psycholinguistic model of reading (later named the top-down or concept-driven model) began to have an impact on views of second language reading. In this model the reader is active, makes predictions, processes information, and reconstruct a message encoded by a writer.
The top-down processing perspective into SL reading had a profound impact on the field, to an extent that it was viewed as a substitute for the bottom-up perspective, rather than its complement.
However, as schema theory research has attempted to make clear, efficient and effective reading (in L1 and L2) requires both top-down and bottom-up strategies operating interactively => Interactive model (Rumelhart 1977). Both top-down and bottom-up processes, functioning interactively, are necessary to an adequate understanding of second language reading and reading comprehension. (Carrell, 1988- pp. 1-4)
A-
The Top Down (Concept-Driven) Approach (Knowledge/background/schemata-based)-
(Goodman,
Smith)
(Overreliance on top-down or knowledge-based processing
=> schema interference)
The "top down" approach emphasizes readers
bringing meaning to text based on their experiential background and interpreting
text based on their prior knowledge (whole language).
Top = higher order mental concepts such as
the knowledge and expectations of the reader.
Bottom = the physical text on the page.
<=> The top-down model of reading focuses
on what the readers bring to the process (Goodman, 1967; Smith, 1971,1982).
The readers sample the text for information and contrast it with their
world knowledge, helping to make sense of what is written. The focus here
is on the readers as they interact with the text.
** This model starts with the hypotheses and predictions then attempts to verify them by working down to the printed stimuli. This view of reading was called the psycholinguistic guessing game.
** According to Goodman, readers employ 5 processes
in reading: (p. 16)
1- Recognition-initiation
2- Prediction
3- Confirmation
4- Correction
5- Termination
Insights that are foundational to this top-down model:
(pp. 12-14)
1- Language, reading included, must be seen in its social context.
2- Competence must be separated from Performance:
Competence = what readers are capable of doing. It results in the
reader's control of and flexibility in using the reading process
Performance = what we observe them to do. It is the observable result
of the competence.
=> Researchers would be committing a serious error if they equated
what readers do with what they are capable of doing.
3- Language must be studied in process.
4- Language must be studied in its human context.
Impact of Goodman's model: (pp. 3, 23, 240)
This model which has recently been characterized as a concept-driven,
top-down pattern had the greatest impact on conceptions about native and
second language reading instruction: it made the reader an active participant
in the reading process => From earlier views of SL reading as a passive
linguistic decoding process to more contemporary views of SL reading as
an active predictive process.
Problems: (Stanovich, 1980)
1- For many texts, the reader has little knowledge
of the topic and cannot generate predictions.
2- Even if a skilled reader can generate predictions,
this would take much longer than it would to recognize the words.
Limitations of top-down models: (Eskey,
1988)
They tend to emphasize higher level skills as the prediction of meaning
by means of context clues or background knowledge at the expense of lower
skills like the rapid and accurate identification of lexical and grammatical
forms.
In making the perfectly valid point that fluent reading is primarily
a cognitive process, they tend to deemphasize the perceptual and decoding
dimensions of that process.
This model is good for the skillful, fluent reader for whom perception
and decoding have become automatic, not for the less proficient, developing
reader.
Good reading is a more language-structured affair than the guessing-game
metaphor seems to imply.
According to Weber (1984), a top-down model of reading is essentially a model of the fluent reader and does not account for all the needs of students who are acquiring reading skills.
Top-Down Applications: (Eskey & Grabe, pp. 229-231)
The content and quantity of texts that second language students are
asked to read may be the most important determinants of whether, and to
what degree, such students develop top-down reading skills.
The materials should be interesting for the students; it should be
assigned in substantial amounts over considerable periods of time.
Two approaches:
- The reading lab approach: students make their own choices
of reading material from among a wide selection of appropriate texts. This
approach allows each student to progress at his own rate, to develop schemata
in some area of interest, and to compile a personal record of reading.
Disadvantage:
it limits group work and isolates reading from other parts of the curriculum.
- The content-centered approach: the teacher provides for interesting
reading in sufficient quantity; a lot of information on a subject for the
class as a whole to explore at some depth.
- pre- and postreading work
(introductory lectures, films, discussions, oral/written presentations.
- student interest is stimulated
- natural blending of skills
(listening, speaking, reading, writing)
- the students collectively
pursue a common goal
- reading is no longer isolated
- reading is no longer taught
as an end in itself but as a means to an end
Disadvantage: loss
of individual choice.
These 2 approaches may be combined within a single program.
B-
The Bottom Up (Serial) Approach
(Text-based)
(LaBerge &
Samuels, MacWorth)
(Overreliance on bottom-up or text-based processing =>
text-boundedness)
The "bottom up" approach stipulates that the
meaning of any text must be "decoded" by the reader and that students are
"reading" when they can "sound out" words on a page. (Phonics)
<=> It emphasizes the ability to de-code
or put into sound what is seen in a text. It ignores helping emerging readers
to recognize what they, as readers, bring to the information on the page.
** This model starts with the printed stimuli
and works its way up to the higher level stages. The sequence of processing
proceeds from the incoming data to higher level encodings.
Problems: (Stanovich, 1980)
- This model has a tendency to depict the information
flow in a series of discrete stages, with each stage transforming the input
and then passing the recorded information on to the next higher stage.
- An important shortcoming of this model is the
fact that it is difficult to account for sentence-context effects and the
role of prior knowledge of text topic as facilitating variables in word
recognition and comprehension (because of lack of feedback).
- According to Eskey (1973), the decoding model is inadequate because it underestimates the contribution of the reader who makes predictions and processes information. It fails to recognize that students utilize their expectations about the text, based on their knowledge of language and how it works. (p. 3)
Bottom-Up Applications: (Eskey & Grabe, pp. 231-236)
Teaching key vocabulary items and, in the area of grammar, teaching
various cohesive devices.
Two areas of concern:
- Simply knowing the meanings of some set number of words does not
ensure that a reader will be able, while reading, to process those words
both rapidly and accurately. => teachers must help students develop identification
skills (exercises for rapid recognition: word recognition and phrase identification
+ extensive reading over time).
- Rate building: good readers read fast; they do not, like many SL
readers, try to read word by word, which destroys their chances of comprehending
very much of the text. => The major bottom-up skill that readers of second
language must acquire is the skill of reading fast. (paced and timed reading
exercises: formal rate-building work should be limited to a few minutes
per class). Major increases in reading ratecan only follow from extensive
reading in the language over time.
Footnote: If a text contains too many difficult
words, no strategy (top down or bottom up) can make such a text accessible
to the reader. However, second language readers do of course encounter
some unknown words in most texts. This is the best means of increasing
their control of English vocabulary. SL readers, however, are frequently
panicked by unknown words, so they stop reading to look them up in dictionaries,
thereby interrupting the normal reading process. In response to this problem,
many SL texts recommend various strategies for guessing the meaning of
unknown words from context, by using semantic and syntactic clues or even
morphological analysis.
In order to develop good reading habits, the best strategy for dealing
with an unknown word may well be to keep reading until the meaning of that
word begins to make itself plain in relation to the larger context provided.
Central to all these bottom-up concerns is the concept of automaticity (LaBerge & Samuels 1974). Good readers process language in the written form of written text without thinking consciously about it, and good SL readers must learn to do so. It is only this kind of automatic processing which allows the good reader to think instead about the larger meaning of the discourse, which allows for global reading with true comprehension.
Bottom-Up Implications for the SL Classroom: (Carrell
p. 240-244)
- Grammatical skills: cohesive devices are very important.
- Vocabulary development:
Vocabulary development and
word recognition have long been recognized as crucial to successful bottom-up
decoding skills. However, schema theory has shed new light on the complex
nature of the interrelationship of schemata, context, and vocabulary knowledge.
UNLIKE traditional views of vocabulary, current thinking converges on the
notion that a given word does not have a fixed meaning, but rather a variety
of meanings that interact with context and background knowledge.
Knowledge of individual word meanings is strongly
associated with conceptual knowledge -- that is, learning vocabulary is
also learning the conceptual knowledge associated with the word. On
the one hand, an important part of teaching background knowledge is teaching
the vocabulary related to it and, conversely, teaching vocabulary may mean
teaching new concepts, new knowledge. Knowledge of vocabulary entails knowledge
of the schemata in which a concept participates, knowledge of the networks
in which that word participates, as well as any associated words and concepts
(=> structural analysis).
Teachers must become aware of the cross-cultural differences in vocabulary and how meaning may be represented differently in the lexicons of various languages.
Several characteristics seem to distinguish effective
from ineffective teaching programs. Preteaching vocabulary in order to
increase learning from text will be more successful
- if the words to be taught are key words in
the target passages
- if the words are taught in semantically and
topically related sets so that word meanings and background knowledge improve
concurrently
- if the words are taught and learned thoroughly
- if both definitional and contextual information
are involved
- if students engage in deeper processing of
word meanings
- if only a few wordsare taught per lesson and
per week.
Research specific to SL reading has shown that merely presenting a list of new or unfamiliar vocabulary items to be encountered in a text, even with definitions appropriate to their use in that text, does not guarantee the learning of the word or the concept behind the word, or of improved reading comprehension on the text passage (Hudson 1982).
To be effective, an extensive and long-term vocabulary
development program accompanying a parallel schemata or background-knowledge-development
program is probably called for. Instead of preteaching vocabulary for single
reading passages, teachers should teach vocabulary and background knowledge
concurrently for sets of passages to be read at some later time.
Every SL curriculum should have a general
program of parallel concept/background knowledge development and vocabulary
development.
C- The Interactive Approach (Rumelhart, Stanovich, Eskey)
For those reading theorists who recognized
the importance of both the text and the reader in the reading process,
an amalgamation of the two emerged—the interactive approach. Reading here
is the process of combining textual information with the information the
reader brings to a text.
The interactive model (Rumelhart 1977; Stanovich
1980) stresses both what is on the written page and what a reader brings
to it using both top-down and bottom-up skills. It views reading is the
interaction between reader and text.
The overreliance on
either mode of processing to the neglect of the other mode has been found
to cause reading difficulties for SL learners (Carrell 1988, p. 239)
The interactive models of reading assume that
skills at all levels are interactively available to process and interpret
the text (Grabe 1988).
In this model, good readers are both good
decoders and good interpreters of text, their decoding skills becoming
more automatic but no less important as their reading skill develops (Eskey
1988).
According to Rumelhart's interactive model:
1- linear models which pass information only
in one direction and which do not permit the information contained in a
higher stage to influence the processing of a lower stage contain a serious
deficiency. Hence the need for an interactive model which permits the information
contained in a higher stage of processing to influence the analysis that
occurs at a lower stage.
2- when an error in word recognition is made,
the word substitution will maintain the same part of speech as the word
for which it was substituted, which will make it difficult for the reader
to understand. (orthographic knowledge)
3- semantic knowledge influences word perception.
(semantic knowledge)
4- perception of syntax for a given word depends
upon the context in which the word is embedded. (syntactic knowledge)
5- our interpretation of what we read depends
upon the context in which a text segment is embedded. (lexical knowledge)
All the aforementioned knowledge sources provide input simultaneously. These sources need to communicate and interact with each other, and the higher-order stages should be able to influence the processing of lower-order stages.
According to Stanovich's interactive-compensatory
model:
* Top-down processing may be easier for the
poor reader who may be slow at word recognition but has knowledge of the
text topic.
* Bottom-up processing may be easier for the
reader who is skilled at word recognition but does not know much about
the text topic.
=> Stanovich's model states, then, that any
stage may communicate with any other and any reader may rely on better
developed knowledge sources when other sources are temporarily weak.
To properly achieve fluency and accuracy, developing
readers must work at perfecting both their bottom-up recognition skills
and their top-down interpretation strategies. Good reading (that is fluent
and accurate reading) can result only from a constant interaction between
these processes.
=> Fluent reading entails both skillful decoding
and relating information to prior knowledge (Eskey, 1988).
<=> Reading is a bi-directional
process that concerns both
the Reader & the
Text.
The level of reader comprehension of the text
is determined by how well the reader variables (interest level in
the text, purpose for reading the text, knowledge of the topic, foreign
language abilities, awareness of the reading process, and level of
willingness to take risks) interact with the
text variables (text type, structure, syntax, and vocabulary) (Hosenfeld,
1979).
http://www.sabes.org/resources/fieldnotes/vol10/f02abrah.htm
According to Joanne Devine (1988), one thing needs to be taken into
consideration: readers' internalized models of the reading process are
extremely important. There is convincing evidence that readers do indeed
have internalized models of the reading process that they bring to bear
when they read.
Sound- or word-centered readers, those who equated good reading with
sound identification or good pronunciation focused their attention on the
graphic information in the text and failed to understand or recall what
they had read.
Meaning-centered readers demonstrated good to excellent recall and
comprehension of text.
=> a reader's theoretical orientations toward reading may determine
the degree to which low proficiency in the language restricts second languagereading
ability.
=> the models that readers hold may be of critical importance in
allowing them to strike a successful balance between bottom-up and top-down
processing necessary for the interpretation of a text.
ESL researchers should be interested in interactive models for several
reasons: (p. 59)
1- several studies note that linguistic deficiencies are inhibiting
factors in reading (Clarke, 1979; Singer, 1981; Carrell, 1988).
2- there is a need for extensive vocabulary for
reading (Alderson and Urquhart, 1984; Singer, 1981)
3- there is a need to account for poor readers who do guess extensively.
4- good readers are not good simply because they are better predictors,
or make better use of context.
Implications of interactive models for ESL reading: (Grabe p.
63)
1- Higher level processing abilities play a significant rolein reading.
2- Many lower-level processing skills are basic
to good reading. => methods of instruction for rapid visual recognition,
for extensive vocabulary development, and for syntactic pattern recognition
should become major pedagogical concerns.
Suggestions for recognition and vocabulary instruction
can be found in Stoller (1984, 1986), McKeown et al. (1985), and Nagy,
Herman, andAnderson (1985).
3- There is a need for a massive receptive vocabulary
that is rapidly, accurately, and automatically accessed -- a fact that
may be the greatest single impediment to fluent reading by ESL students.
4- Students may overcompensate (overrely on text
or on context) for a lack of relevant schemata; Simple analyses of student
difficulties which explain all problems as word-boundedness, or as unwillingness
to guess or take chances, are not justified by the range of empirical studies
in the literature.
5- The development of reading abilities may be
viewed more profitably if seen in terms of stages of skills development.
Some implications of the interactive model of reading for ESL:
(Eskey & Grabe, p. 225)
- contextual interpretation of lexical items is only a part of the
vocabulary skills needed for fluent reading, and may actually interfere
if a student overrelies on this sraqtegy (Stanovich 1980).
- certain kinds of "phonics" exercises may be helpful to students (Beck
1981).
- basic recognition exercises to improve speed and accuracy of perception
may constitute an important component of an effective second language reading
program (Stoller 1984).
Some general implications for the teaching
of second language reading: (Eskey &
Grabe, p. 227)
1- Some time must be devoted in the reading class
to bottom-up concerns such as the rapid and accurate identification of
lexical and grammatical forms. Even students who have developed strong
top-down skills in their native languages may not be able to transfer these
higher-level skills to a SL context until they have developed a stronger
bottom-up foundation of basic identification skills.
2- Some time must also be devoted in the reading class to top-down
concerns such as
- reading for global meaning
(as opposed to mere decoding),
- developing a willingness
to take chances
- developing appropriate
schemata for the proper interpretation of texts.
Reading of any kind of text must be treated as real reading, that is,
reading for meaning. No student should ever be forced or encouraged to
limit him/herself to decoding skills.
In short, for second language readers, especially, both top-down and bottom-up skills and strategies must be developed conjointly since both contribute directly to the successful comprehension of text.
Short Circuit Hypothesis:
Goodman, p. 16:
Any reading that does not end with meaning is a short circuit. In general,
readers short circuit when
- they cannot get meaning or lose the structure;
- they use non-productive reading strategies;
- they are not permitted to terminate non-productive reading.
List of short circuits:
- letter naming
- recoding
- syntactic nonsense
- partial structures
Clarke p. 120:
The results of some studies conducted suggest that the role of language
proficiency may be greater than has previously been assumed: limited
control over the language "short circuits" the good reader's system causing
him/her to revert to poor reader strategies when confronted with a difficult
task in the second language. => This suggests that it may be inaccurate
to speak of "good readers" and "poor readers," but of good or poor reading
behaviors which characterize most readers at different times. When one
is confronted with difficult reading, one is likely to revert to poor reading
behaviors.
Some of the implications of the "short circuit hypothesis" for ESL
reading teachers:
1- It would seem justifiable to develop reading programs that are
characteristic of good readers.
Among the behaviors that seem to be most productive
are:
- concentrating on passage-level
semantic cues
- formulating hypotheses
about the text beforereading, then reading to confirm,
refine
or reject those hypotheses
- deemphasizing graphophonic
and syntactic accuracy => developing a tolerance for inexactness,
a willingness
to take chances and make mistakes.
2- The results of these studies stress the importance of language skills
for effective reading. This finding supports the activities of "traditional"
teachers (Lado, 1964; Finochiaro, 1974) whose approach to teaching reading
emphasided grammar lessons and vocabulary instruction; it also supports
the recent attempts to integrate reading skills and language development
(Eskey, 1973; Baudoin et al, 1977; Silberstein, 1977).
=> ESL teachers need to emphasize the need for guessing and taking
chances in addition to helping their students acquire fundamental language
skills that would facilitate the process of reading. They should emphasize
both the psycho and the linguistic.
The Importance of Vocabulary (Eskey &
Grabe, p. 226)
All models of reading recognize the importance
of vocabulary, but the interactive model goes further. Not only is a large
vocabulary important, it is a prerequisite to fluent reading skills.
Since automatic word recognition is more important
to fluent processing of text than context clues as a first strategy, large-scale
development of recognition vocabulary is crucial (Perfetti 1985).
The importance of vocabulary is not only related
to the number of words, but also to the number of times that these words
are encountered and retrieved in texts.
Conclusion (Eskey & Grabe, pp. 228-229)
We must make a clear distinction between the building up of particular
skills and strategies, or of relevant knowledge, and reading itself. Both
top-down and bottom-up skills can, in the long run, only be developed by
extensive reading over time. Classroom work can point the way but cannot
substitute for the act itself: people learn to read by reading, not by
doing exercises.
What is needed is:
- extensive reading
- appropriate materials (relevant to students' needs and interests)
- sound teacher judgment and approach (the teacher will determine how
much and what his/her students read; the teacher must create the world
of reading in class; the teacher must stimulate interest in reading; the
teacher must project his/her enthusiasm for books; the teacher must help
students to see that reading can be of real value to them; the teacher
must choose, edit, modify or create materials for students; the teacher
must introduce, and provide practice in, useful reading strategies for
coping with texts in an unfamiliar language; the teacher must provide students
with feedback as needed).
Language Competence & L2 Reading Proficiency (Devine
1988, pp. 266-268)
The general findings of research -- that low
reading achievement in a SL is significantly related to low general proficiency
in that language and that readers with low L2 language proficiency are
especially handicapped in their ability to utilize contextual constraints
and cohesive devices when reading in the target language -- have led some
researchers to suggest that there is a threshold of linguistic competence
necessary for successful L2 reading ("linguistic ceiling" according to
Clarke 1980). => L2 readers will not be able to read effectively until
they develop some proficiency in the target language (TL).
Grabe (1986) contends that successful L2 reading
depends upon the procession of a "critical mass of knowledge" : linguistic
knowledge (automatic processing of syntactic patterns and vocabulary) +
background knowledge + schematic knowledge (relevant formal and content
schemata).
In the last 25 years, the field of second language acquisition has seen renewed interest in vocabulary learning and acquisition. There are many dimensions to vocabulary learning and acquisition, as reflected in the multitude of different areas of research being done on the topic.
Research into Learning Words in Context vs. Learning Words Out of Context
There exist conflicting views among language professionals concerning
the relative superiority of two approaches to learning second language
vocabulary: learning words in context vs. learning words out of context.
Convictions are strong among many language professionals that contextualized
vocabulary learning is more effective than learning words in lists. Oxford
and Scarcella (1994), for example, observe that while decontexutalized
learning (word lists) may help students memorize vocabulary for tests,
students are likely to rapidly forget words memorized from lists. McCarthy
(1990) argues that a word learned in a meaningful context is best assimilated
and remembered. However, most studies have failed to produce findings favoring
context-dependent vocabulary learning (e.g. Morgan and Bailey, 1943; Wind
and Davidson, 1969; Gershman, 1970, Tudor and Hafiz, 1989, Hulstjin, 1992
).
Moreover, in recent literature dealing with vocabulary acquisition,
there can be seen increasing advocacy for explicitly teaching words out
of context at an early stage of language acquisition, with more context-based
vocabulary learning taking place at later stages of language development
(e.g. Coady, 1997b; Meara, 1997; Nation and Newton, 1997). To justify their
position, these advocates often draw attention to the paradoxical situation
facing the novice L2 (second language) learner of having to learn vocabulary
through extensive reading or listening when they don’t know enough words
to read or listen well. This suggests the logical importance of helping
beginners explicitly learn the basic 3,000 word families, thought to represent
the fundamental lexical competence by which learners can read independently
and acquire language in a natural manner (Laufer, 1997).
While the debate concerning learning words in context vs. learning
words out of context has yet to be resolved, a potentially effective third
approach to teaching vocabulary is beginning to emerge through research
findings. This approach combines decontextualized vocabulary instruction
with contextualized reading. Two studies have found learning that involves
both contextual reading and explicit vocabulary instruction results in
much greater gains in vocabulary knowledge than does contextualized learning
through reading alone (Paribakht and Wesche, 1997; Zimmerman, 1994).
Sannoui’s research (1992, 1995) identified two distinctive approaches to L2 vocabulary learning: those who structured their vocabulary learning, independently engaged in a variety of learning activities and practiced target words, and those that did not. Learners with a structured approach were shown to be more successful than those who followed an unstructured approach, regardless of level of instruction or type of instruction received.
Second language teachers need to think of ways of exposing ‘poorer’
learners to the ways that ‘good’ learners approach lexical learning. That
is, making ‘poorer’ learners more conscious of the need to develop a more
independent and structured approach to vocabulary learning, which research
has shown to be most associated with success in vocabulary learning.
Introducing and having learners practice using a variety of alternative
vocabulary learning strategies can be considered an effective way of enabling
learners to achieve more effective independent vocabulary learning in the
future. Research has shown that strategies involving deeper elaboration
(i.e. more active processing of information) result in better retention
of words. However, many mnemonic and non-mnemonic semantic elaboration
strategies assume a reasonable L2 vocabulary base from which associations
can be made. For this reason, instruction in such strategies should be
considered largely ineffective for beginner-level L2 learners, but would
benefit learners of higher proficiency levels.
http://college.hmco.com/instructors/ins_teachtech_foundations_module_vocdev_printerfriendly.html
Because a reader's comprehension of words predicts the reader's comprehension
of text, it is important that students learn vocabulary.
*Extensive reading can increase direct vocabulary knowledge, but direct
instruction that engages the student in the construction of word meaning
using context and prior knowledge is effective for learning specific vocabulary
and for improving comprehension of related materials. Nelson-Herber, Joan.
"Expanding and refining vocabulary in content areas." Journal of Reading,
April, 1986: 626-633.
*The most effective vocabulary instruction is the kind that also improves
comprehension. An effective program included: directing students to self-select
words directly related to important elements of a selection; teaching the
words in context; and providing multiple sessions of practice using the
words.
Dole, Janice, et al. "Teaching vocabulary within the context of literature."
Journal of Reading, March, 1995: 452-460.
*Recent research repeatedly recognizes these seven topics as critical:
IV. Implementation
A. Exploration exercises for instructor
Activity: Use context to unlock meaning.
Define gyxpx.
You say you can't do it?
Well, then here it is in a sentence. Perhaps context will help.
The gyxpx was broken.
What? You still don't know it? OK, here is some more contexts.
The new secretary was fast as the blazes on the gyxpx.
Ah, now you have an idea, right? It might be some type of word processor,
or a calculator, or even a photocopy machine. Would it help if you knew
the gyxpx has a shift key that keeps sticking? That makes it pretty clear
that the gyxpx is something with a keyboard that is used in an office.
The strategy that helped you figure it out was the use of context clues.
There won't always be helpful context clues for a word, but it's a good
place to start when a word is unknown.
Activity: Use structure to unlock meaning.
If the Greek word PHIL means love
BIBLIO means book
Then what does BIBLIOPHILE mean? It is a lover of books.
If the Greek word GRAPH means write
Then what does BIBLIOGRAPHY mean? It is a written list of books.
If the Greek word CHOR means dance Then what does CHOREOGRAPH
mean? It is the writing of a story in dance.
If you understand the structure of a word, you may be able to use it
to decipher its meaning. Word elements can be part of a strategy.
Activity: Use a dictionary to unlock meaning.
You are now a History professor. Ask your students to define occupation.
a. an activity in which one engages
b. the principal business of one's life
c. the holding and control of an area by a foreign military force
You are now a Psychology professor. Ask your students to define ego.
a. the self especially as contrasted with another self or the
world
b. self-esteem
c. the conscious part of the personality that is derived from
the id through contacts with reality
You are now an English professor. Tell your students they are responsible
for knowing this term: climax.
a. a figure of speech in which a series of phrases or sentences
is arranged in ascending order of rhetorical forcefulness
b. the highest point
c. the point of highest dramatic tension or a major turning point
in the action (as of a play)
The addition of some context may help them to understand these terms,
but they probably think they already know them. Where can you direct them
to get the most precise meaning? Teach them how to use a dictionary effectively
and you will make a great contribution to their knowledge of vocabulary.
A personal dictionary requires students to self-select the terms to
be learned. Direct students to copy the unfamiliar word in its entire context,
followed by: its pronunciation, part of speech, definition according to
how it is used in the sentence, and a sample sentence showing their understanding
of the word.
English language learners may bring linguistic knowledge in the area of cognates to their learning of new English words, but they also will have special vocabulary learning needs that English speakers will not. They need to learn basic, survival words that English speakers begin school knowing, words such as house, school, walk, and eat. They also need to learn the multiple meanings of many of these basic words. English lacks many of the morphemic markers that other languages have that can indicate what part of speech it is.
In its comprehensive review and analysis of 30 years of reading research, the National Reading Panel described five main methods of teaching vocabulary: (1) explicit instruction, such as providing definitions; (2) implicit instruction (exposure as students read widely); (3) multimedia methods, such as graphic representations and hypertext; (4) capacity methods in which practice is emphasized to make reading automatic; and (5) association methods in which learners draw connections between known and unknown words.
Estimates of the number of words that the average high school senior
knows range from a
high of 50,000 to a low of 17,000 (Nagy and Anderson, 1984; D’Anna
et al., 1991). This
translates to learning 3,000 to 4,000 new words a year for English
speakers.
Second language learners have an even greater vocabulary acquisition
task in front of them
(García, 2003; Hirsh and Nation, 1992).
Students from low-income backgrounds also tend to have limited vocabularies,
especially for
academic terms. Hart and Risley estimated a 30-million word gap by
age three between the
average number of words heard by the children of parents on welfare
and those whose
parents are professionals (2003).
Graves and Watts-Taffe recommend four components to any vocabulary instruction
program: wide reading so that implicit learning can occur; teaching
individual words;
teaching word learning strategies; and fostering word consciousness.
The teaching of
individual words is “most effective when learners are given both definitional
and contextual
information, when learners actively process the new word meanings,
and when they
experience multiple encounters with words” (2002).
Recommended ways to teach word learning strategies include using
context, using word
parts to unlock the meanings of unknown words, and teaching students
how to use the
dictionary.
English language learners may bring linguistic knowledge in the area
of cognates to their
learning of new English words, but they also will have special vocabulary
learning needs that
English speakers will not.
They need to learn basic, survival words that English speakers begin
school knowing, words
such as house, school, walk, and eat. They also need to learn the multiple
meanings of many
of these basic words.
Graves and Watts-Taffe recommend four components to any vocabulary instruction
program: wide reading so that implicit learning can occur; teaching
individual words;
teaching word learning strategies; and fostering word consciousness.
The teaching of
individual words is “most effective when learners are given both definitional
and contextual
information, when learners actively process the new word meanings,
and when they
experience multiple encounters with words” (2002).
Recommended ways to teach word learning strategies include using context,
using word
parts to unlock the meanings of unknown words, and teaching students
how to use the
dictionary.
Word consciousness is “awareness of and interest in words and their
meanings… and
integrates metacognition about words and motivation for learning words.”
(McKeown and
Beck, in press).
In their study, McKeown and Beck, for example, had teachers keep Word
Wizard Charts to
encourage students to become more word conscious. Students were challenged
to find
outside of class the words they had been taught in class in books,
newspapers, radio,
television, and adult conversation – and were rewarded for bringing
in examples of how the
words were used.
McKeown and Beck say that a mature, literate individual’s vocabulary
comprises three tiers.
Tier One has the most basic words, such as bed, brother, sky, and run.
Most learners already
know the meanings of Tier One words.
Tier Three consists of rare words such as apogee, precinct, peninsula
and ecclesiastical which tend to be limited to specific domains.
Tier Two, in the middle, consists of words like compromise, scrutinize,
diligent and typical
which are of high frequency for mature users and found across a variety
of domains.
The researchers found that instruction directed at Tier Two words is
the most productive.
Their vocabulary development program helps teachers determine which
words to teach and
to what depth as well as showing them how to directly teach students
to use context
efficiently. It incorporates a number of innovative vocabulary development
techniques that
they call “rich instruction” to produce “deep and facile word knowledge…
needed to affect
comprehension” including ones called Word Wizard as described above,
Overheard
Conversations, and Word Lines for Tier Two words.
A- Word Recognition Strategies
1- Sight words
2- Phonics
http://www.eduplace.com/rdg/res/phonic.html
3- Context clues: Students learn to quickly find the main idea by skimming and surveying the text for headings, graphic materials, and terms in boldface that can provide context clues.
4- Structural analysis
5- Configuration/ Visual clues
6- Dictionary
It pays to be patient. Don't reach for the dictionary as soon as you see an unfamiliar word. Read the whole sentence. The meaning of the unfamilar word may become obvious from context or you may conclude that you have comprehended enough not to have to bother with looking it up. There is always a good chance that clues to a word's meaning may appear later in the paragraph because writers often try to help their readers understand by giving additional explanations, definitions, and clarifications.
http://www.public.asu.edu/~ickpl/Reading_Strategies.htm
http://coe.fgcu.edu/faculty/ray/red/cstrategies.htm
http://www.manatee.k12.fl.us/sites/elementary/palmasola/rvocabindex.htm
7- Applying various forms of word recognition strategies to text materials
http://www.eduplace.com/rdg/res/teach/
http://www.ldonline.org/ld_indepth/reading/ldrp_chard_guidelines.html
http://www.bankstreet.edu/literacyguide/tech.html
Beginning readers learn to use several clues or problem solving strategies to figure out unknown words and to make sense of written texts. Individual learners develop and use these techniques in varied ways and at different times. Emergent readers and writers generally start out using just a few strategies, such as looking at pictures and using memory, and as they become more fluent, they add more. As a tutor it is important to see what a child already knows and does use so that you can build on that knowledge and extend it. Ultimately the goal is to help readers learn and integrate the whole range of strategies, so that they do not overuse just one.
MEMORY - PICTURES - CONTEXT - VISUAL CLUES - PHONICS - STRUCTURAL ANALYSIS
MEMORY: Emergent and Early Readers successfully use their memory of story and specific language patterns to help them "approximate" or "role play" reading. This is an important first step. That is why patterned stories, and repeated reading of engaging texts are successful for these very beginning readers.
PICTURE CLUES: Most children will be attracted by illustrations, and as beginning readers they should be encouraged to use that source of information as they try to make sense of print. Emergent readers will use pictures and memory to construct the story, with little attention to the print; gradually they will use the pictures to help them predict and confirm individual words in the text. Eventually, as they develop other strategies and become more fluent readers, they will rely less on the pictures. However, even fluent readers, should be encouraged to create mental images, to visualize, as they read texts with fewer illustrations.
CONTEXT CLUES: Successful readers use the surrounding ideas and words of a sentence, as well as their own background knowledge, and sense of language to figure out words, and to understand text. For example: "Do you know ---- time it is?" is easily read by using the surrounding words to predict the missing one. Early Fluent and Fluent readers, too, should be taught to use this strategy, building on their expectation that written language will make sense and sound like spoken language.
Direct Definition Context Clues are words that say, “Stop - don’t
touch that dictionary. The definition of the word you don’t know is right
here in the text!" They are often used in content area reading. Content
area reading is reading that you do in different subject areas such as
mathematics, social studies and science.
http://www.manatee.k12.fl.us/sites/elementary/palmasola/ccdirect.htm
VISUAL CLUES: In addition to pictures, early fluent and fluent readers learn to use the configuration of words (length, shape, specific visual details) to recognize whole words. Gradually, too, they notice patterns of letters within words to figure out new words (ear in hear; -ing in thing, swing...). Caution: While successful readers will eventually learn to recognize many, many words instantly, especially exciting content words like "elephant" and "Exterminator" some of the more common words (high frequency words) such as "the", "this", "what", "who", "then", "there" are much harder to learn because they look so much alike to a new reader. Rather than drilling these and other similar words in isolation, they can be better taught and read within the context of whole sentences (i.e.: "Who is that at the door? " "What time is it?") (See Sample Games)
PHONETIC CLUES: Readers use knowledge of letter sound associations, especially initial and ending consonants to help them figure out words. This is sometimes called "sounding out" and works best when it is combined with the use of context or picture clues. Tutors can help Emergent and Early readers develop "Phonemic Awareness" (awareness of the sounds and sound patterns of spoken words) through a variety of reading and writing activities and through games (see: Sample Games; Suggested Books, Writing Activities).
Early Fluent and Fluent Readers and writers gradually learn to distinguish more sounds within words and to apply that knowledge in reading and writing; they are increasingly aware of the middle sounds, or sound patterns such as "tion".
Caution: Although phonemic awareness is important for reading, mastery of phonic knowledge is not a prerequisite; many children gain knowledge of the sound system of language in the course of listening to and reading meaningful books, and through opportunities to write. Furthermore, while phonetic clues are useful as one technique, it is usually not very efficient to sound out entire words. Phonetic clues work best when combined with other strategies.
Also, remember that beginning readers should not be expected to sound out words such as names or concept words that are not already part of their spoken vocabulary.
STRUCTURAL ANALYSIS (Word Parts): Early Fluent and Fluent Readers can use their increased awareness of the structure of words (word parts) to help figure out new words. They can be helped to notice roots and endings (play, played, playing; fast, faster, fastest) and suffixes and prefixes (un/help/ful). They also can learn about "compound words" (some/thing, every/body).
Remember: No single element works all the time for everyone. Successful
readers use different combinations of strategies and word analysis skills.
Some approaches are easier for some readers than others. That is why it
is essential to help beginning readers learn to use all the approaches
as they become ready to do so.
Vocabulary
In order for students to comprehend what they are reading, the words must make sense to them. The links provided below discuss various ways in which vocabulary lessons can be used before, during, and after reading.
Links:
F Scholastic page Includes two explicit lessons on vocabulary connections and word webs. It also includes a vocabulary recording sheet to use during lessons. http://teacher.scholastic.com/lessonrepro/lessonplans/instructor/readclin4.htm
F Bigchalk, brought to you by The Education Network provides direct access to a host of creative games, ideas and lessons for teaching vocabulary based comprehension. From primary to high school, these activities are sure to be a hit for your students.
http://www.bigchalk.com/cgi-bin/WebObjects/WOPortal.woa/918/wa/HWCDA/hwcbetween?search=vocabulary&underSID=6404&7.14.7.5.0.1.x=120&7.14.7.5.0.1.y=10
F This site focuses on vocabulary building through the use of prefixes, suffixes, contractions, homophones, synonyms, antonyms, and compounds. The printable practice sheets come with lessons to use in the classroom. http://www.manatee.k12.fl.us/sites/elementary/palmasola/rvocabindex.htm
F Provides definitions and explanations of learning strategies to help improve vocabulary development through linguistic, spatial, visual, and physical mnemonics, semantic mapping, and other means. http://www.public.asu.edu/~ickpl/learningvocab.htm
http://coe.fgcu.edu/faculty/ray/red/cstrategies.htm
Title: An Investigation of Three Methods of Teaching Vocabulary at the
Junior High Level.
Authors: Geiger, Evangeline Drury;
Publication Date: 1967-00-00
Pages: 174
Abstract: This study of methods of teaching vocabulary in the junior high school investigated three approaches: use of a programed text in vocabulary development that emphasized context clues, use of the programed text augmented by listening assistance, and use of the programed text augmented by a word-analysis supplement. Over 300 students with I.Q.'s between 75 and 125 (3 experimental groups and a control group) were tested by comparing pre- and post-tests in reading, vocabulary development, and the ability to generalize word meanings. During the 6 weeks of the study, the experimental groups devoted 15 minutes a day to vocabulary development using the programed text or the programed text with a designated supplement. Analysis of covariance, tests of significance, and correlation techniques were used in the treatment of data. Results indicated that vocabularies of seventh-grade students can be enlarged by using a programed text emphasizing context clues, that listening assistance contributes to retention and is an effective reinforcement for programed instruction in developing vocabulary and comprehension, and that a word-analysis supplement does not contribute to the effectiveness of a programed text in vocabulary development emphasizing context clues. (Author/JS)